Small Navigation Menu

Primary Menu

Concept Appraisal

Citation: Shea, Nicholas and Smortchkova, Joulia and Quilty-dunn, James and Thorne, Saphire and Hampton, James (2021) Concept Appraisal. Cognitive Science, 45 (5). ISSN 1551-6709

Thorne et al_Concept Appraisal_CogSci21 (1).pdf

Creative Commons: Attribution-No Derivative Works 4.0

This paper reports the first empirical investigation of the hypothesis that epistemic appraisals form part of the structure of concepts. To date, studies of concepts have focused on the way concepts encode properties of objects and the way those features are used in categorization and in other cognitive tasks. Philosophical considerations show the importance of also considering how a thinker assesses the epistemic value of beliefs and other cognitive resources and, in particular, concepts. We demonstrate that there are multiple, reliably judged, dimensions of epistemic appraisal of concepts. Four of these dimensions are accounted for by a common underlying factor capturing how well people believe they understand a concept. Further studies show how dimensions of concept appraisal relate to other aspects of concepts. First, they relate directly to the hierarchical organization of concepts, reflecting the increase in specificity from superordinate to basic and subordinate levels. Second, they predict inductive choices in category-based induction. Our results suggest that epistemic appraisals of concepts form a psychologically important yet previously overlooked aspect of the structure of concepts. These findings will be important in understanding why individuals sometimes abandon and replace certain concepts; why social groups do so, for example, during a “scientific revolution”; and how we can facilitate such changes when we engage in deliberate “conceptual engineering” for epistemic, social, and political purposes.

Creators: Shea, Nicholas (0000-0002-2032-5705) and Smortchkova, Joulia and Quilty-dunn, James and Thorne, Saphire and Hampton, James and
DOI: https://doi.org/10.1111/cogs.12978
Official URL: https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/cogs.1...
Subjects: Philosophy
Divisions: Institute of Philosophy
Collections: London Philosophy Papers
Dates:
  • 24 March 2021 (accepted)
  • 21 May 2021 (published)
References: Ashby, F. G., & Valentin, V. V. (2017). Multiple systems of perceptual category learning: Theory and cognitive tests. In H. Cohen & C. Lefebvre (Eds.), Handbook of categorization in cognitive science ( 2nd ed., pp. 157– 188). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Crossref Google Scholar Boyd, R. (1991). Realism, anti-foundationism and the enthusiasm for natural kinds. Philosophical Studies, 61, 127– 148. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Cappelen, H. (2018). Fixing language. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crossref Google Scholar Carey, S. (2009). The origin of concepts. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crossref Google Scholar Carr, J. R. (2017). Epistemic utility theory and the aim of belief. Philosophy and Phenomenological Research, 95(3), 511– 534. Wiley Online Library Web of Science®Google Scholar Corter, J. E., & Gluck, M. A. (1992). Explaining basic categories: Feature predictability and information. Psychological Bulletin, 111(2), 291. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Dahlgren, K. (1985). The cognitive structure of social categories. Cognitive Science, 9(3), 379– 398. Wiley Online Library Web of Science®Google Scholar Davidson, D. (1986). A coherence theory of knowledge and truth. In D. Davidson (Ed.), Truth and interpretation (pp. 307– 319). New York: Oxford University Press. Google Scholar Davies, M. (2015). Knowledge–-Explicit, implicit and tacit: Philosophical aspects. In J. D. Wright (Ed.), International encyclopedia of the social & behavioral sciences (pp. 74– 90). Amsterdam: Elsevier. Crossref Google Scholar Dunbar, K. (1997). How scientists think: On-line creativity and conceptual change in science. In T. B. Ward, S. M. Smith, & J. Vaid (Eds.), Creative thought: An investigation of conceptual structures and processes (pp. 461– 493). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association Press. Crossref Google Scholar Égré, P., & Bonnay, D. (2012). Metacognitive perspectives on unawareness and uncertainty. In M. Beran, J. Brandl, J. Perner, & J. Proust (Eds.), Foundations of metacognition (pp. 321– 342). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crossref Google Scholar Égré, P., & O'Madagáin, C. (2019). Concept utility. Journal of Philosophy, 116(10), 525– 554. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Fleming, S. M., & Daw, N. D. (2017). Self-evaluation of decision-making: A general Bayesian framework for metacognitive computation. Psychological Review, 124(1), 91– 114. Crossref PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Fleming, S. M., & Lau, H. C. (2014). How to measure metacognition. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, 443. Crossref PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Fleming, S. M., Weil, R. S., Nagy, Z., Dolan, R. J., & Rees, G. (2010). Relating introspective accuracy to individual differences in brain structure. Science, 329(5998), 1541– 1543. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1191883 Crossref CAS PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Goldman, A. I. (1978). Epistemics: The regulative theory of cognition. Journal of Philosophy, 75(10), 509– 523. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Goldman, A. I. (1999). Knowledge in a social world. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crossref Google Scholar Goldman, A. I., & Beddor, B. (2015). Reliabilist epistemology. In E. N. Zalta (Ed.), The Stanford encyclopedia of philosophy. Retrieved from <https://plato.stanford.edu/archives/win2016/entries/reliabilism/> Google Scholar Hampton, J. A., & Gardiner, M. M. (1983). Measures of internal category structure: A correlational analysis of normative data. British Journal of Psychology, 74(4), 491– 516. Wiley Online Library Web of Science®Google Scholar Haslam, N., Rothschild, L., & Ernst, D. (2000). Essentialist beliefs about social categories. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39(1), 113– 127. Wiley Online Library PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Haslanger, S. (2000). Gender and race:(What) are they?(What) do we want them to be? Nous, 34(1), 31– 55. Wiley Online Library Web of Science®Google Scholar Hookway, C. (1994). Cognitive virtues and epistemic evaluations. International Journal of Philosophical Studies, 2(2), 211– 227. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Jacoby, L. L., Wahlheim, C. N., & Coane, J. H. (2010). Test-enhanced learning of natural concepts: Effects on recognition memory, classification, and metacognition. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 36(6), 1441– 1451. Crossref PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Kalish, C. W. (2015). Normative concepts. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds.), The conceptual mind: New directions in the study of concepts (pp. 519– 540). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar Keil, F. C. (1989). Concepts, kinds, and conceptual development. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Google Scholar Keil, F. C., & Batterman, N. (1984). A characteristic-to-defining shift in the development of word meaning. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 23(2), 221– 236. Crossref Google Scholar Knobe, J., Prasada, S., & Newman, G. E. (2013). Dual character concepts and the normative dimension of conceptual representation. Cognition, 127(2), 242– 257. Crossref PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Koenig, M. A., & Harris, P. L. (2005). Preschoolers mistrust ignorant and inaccurate speakers. Child Development, 76(6), 1261– 1277. Wiley Online Library PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Kominsky, J. F., Zamm, A. P., & Keil, F. C. (2018). Knowing when help is needed: A developing sense of causal complexity. Cognitive Science, 42(2), 491– 523. Wiley Online Library PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Koriat, A. (2007). Metacognition and consciousness. In P. D. Zelazo, M. Moscovitch, & E. Thompson (Eds.), Cambridge handbook of consciousness (pp. 289– 325). Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Koriat, A. (2011). Subjective confidence in perceptual judgments: A test of the self-consistency model. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 140, 117– 139. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022171 Crossref PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Koriat, A. (2012). The self-consistency model of subjective confidence. Psychological Review, 119, 80– 113. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0025648 Crossref PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Koriat, A. (2015). Metacognition: Decision making processes in self-monitoring and self-regulation. In G. Keren & G. Wu (Eds.), The Wiley Blackwell handbook of judgment and decision making (Vol. 1, pp. 356– 379). Malden, MA: John Wiley & Sons. Wiley Online Library Google Scholar Kuhn, T. S. (1970). The structure of scientific revolutions. Chicago: University of Chicago. Web of Science®Google Scholar López, A., Atran, S., Coley, J. D., Medin, D., & Smith, E. E. (1997). The tree of life: Universal and cultural features of folkbiological taxonomies and inductions. Cognitive Psychology, 32, 251– 295. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Machery, E. (2017). Philosophy within its proper bounds. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crossref Google Scholar Millikan, R. G. (2000). On clear and confused ideas. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Crossref Google Scholar Mills, C. M., & Keil, F. C. (2004). Knowing the limits of one's understanding: The development of an awareness of an illusion of explanatory depth. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 87(1), 1– 32. Crossref PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Murphy, G. L. (2002). The big book of concepts. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Crossref Google Scholar Murphy, G., & Medin, D. (1985). The role of theories in conceptual coherence. Psychological Review, 92(3), 289– 316. Crossref CAS PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Nelson, L. J., & Miller, D. T. (1995). The distinctiveness effect in social categorization: You are what makes you unusual. Psychological Science, 6(4), 246– 249. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Newman, G. E., & Knobe, J. (2019). The essence of essentialism. Mind and Language, 34(5), 585– 605. https://doi.org/10.1111/mila.12226 Wiley Online Library Web of Science®Google Scholar Osherson, D. N., Smith, E. E., Wilkie, O., López, A., & Shafir, E. (1990). Category-based induction. Psychological Review, 97, 185– 200. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Patalano, A. L., Chin-Parker, S., & Ross, B. H. (2006). The importance of being coherent: Category coherence, cross-classification, and reasoning. Journal of Memory and Language, 54(3), 407– 424. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Patalano, A. L., Wengrovitz, S. M., & Sharpes, K. M. (2009). The influence of category coherence on inference about cross-classified entities. Memory & Cognition, 37(1), 21– 28. Crossref PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Proust, J. (2012). Metacognition and mindreading: One or two functions? In M. Beran, J. Brandl, J. Perner, & J. Proust (Eds.), Foundations of metacognition (pp. 234– 251). Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crossref Google Scholar Proust, J. (2013). The philosophy of metacognition: Mental agency and self-awareness. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crossref Google Scholar Putnam, H. (1973). Meaning and reference. Journal of Philosophy, 70(19), 699– 711. Crossref Google Scholar Quine, W. V. (1961). Two dogmas of empiricism. From a logical point of view. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Web of Science®Google Scholar Quine, W. V. (1970). Natural kinds. In N. Rescher (Ed), Essays in honor of Carl G. Hempel (pp. 1– 23). Dordrecht: D. Reidel. Google Scholar Rehder, B., & Hastie, R. (2004). Category coherence and category-based property induction. Cognition, 91(2), 113– 153. Crossref PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Rips, L. J. (1975). Inductive judgments about natural categories. Journal of Memory and Language, 14(6), 665. Google Scholar Rosch, E. (1977). Human categorization. In N. Warren (Ed.), Studies in cross-cultural psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 177– 206). London: Academic Press. Google Scholar Rosch, E., Mervis, C. B., Gray, W. D., Johnson, D. M., & Boyes-Braem, P. (1976). Basic objects in natural categories. Cognitive Psychology, 8(3), 382– 439. Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Rozenblit, L., & Keil, F. (2002). The misunderstood limits of folk science: An illusion of explanatory depth. Cognitive Science, 26(5), 521– 562. Wiley Online Library PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Skorupski, J. (2010). The domain of reasons. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Crossref Google Scholar Sloutsky, V. M. (2010). From perceptual categories to concepts: What develops? Cognitive Science, 34(7), 1244– 1286. Wiley Online Library PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Smortchkova, J., & Shea, N. (2020). Metacognitive development and conceptual change in children. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 11, 745– 763. https://doi.org/10.1007/s13164-020-00477-7 Crossref Web of Science®Google Scholar Sperber, D. (2010). The guru effect. Review of Philosophy and Psychology, 1(4), 583– 592. Crossref Google Scholar Sperber, D., Clement, F., Heintz, C., Mascaro, O., Mercier, H., Origgi, G., & Wilson, D. (2010). Epistemic vigilance. Mind & Language, 25(4), 359– 393. Wiley Online Library Web of Science®Google Scholar Tabachnick, B. G., & Fidell, L. S. (2007). Using multivariate statistics ( 5th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Allyn & Bacon. Google Scholar Thomasson, A. L. (2017). Metaphysics and conceptual negotiation. Philosophical Issues, 27(1), 364– 382. Wiley Online Library Web of Science®Google Scholar Treanor, N. (2013). The measure of knowledge. Nous, 47(3), 577– 601. Wiley Online Library Web of Science®Google Scholar Tversky, B., & Hemenway, K. (1984). Objects, parts, and categories. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 113(2), 169. Crossref CAS PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar Wahlheim, C. N., Finn, B., & Jacoby, L. L. (2012). Metacognitive judgments of repetition and variability effects in natural concept learning: Evidence for variability neglect. Memory & Cognition, 40, 703– 716. Crossref PubMed Web of Science®Google Scholar

Statistics

View details